Explanations for Forgetting

This section explores explanations for forgetting in Psychology. Forgetting refers to the loss or failure to retrieve information from memory when it is needed. Several explanations for forgetting exist within psychology, with interference theory and retrieval failure being two key approaches. Understanding these explanations helps explain how and why memories are not always accessible and the specific conditions under which forgetting is more likely to occur.

Interference Theory of Forgetting

Interference theory proposes that forgetting occurs because different memories interfere with each other, particularly when the information is similar. The theory identifies two types of interference: proactive interference and retroactive interference.

Proactive Interference (PI)

Proactive interference (PI) occurs when older memories disrupt the recall of newer information. In other words, previous learning interferes with the acquisition or retrieval of new information, especially when the old and new material are similar.

Key Features:

Explanation: PI suggests that the memory trace of previously learned information is still strong, making it difficult to store or retrieve new, similar information.

Example: If you have learned Spanish in the past and then start learning French, you may accidentally recall Spanish words when trying to speak or remember French vocabulary.

Evidence: Underwood (1957) reviewed studies and found that participants often recalled fewer items when they had been required to remember multiple lists of similar material, as prior lists interfered with the recall of later ones.

Retroactive Interference (RI)

Retroactive interference (RI) happens when newer memories interfere with the recall of older information. This means that newly learned information disrupts previously stored memories, making it difficult to recall earlier material.

Key Features:

Explanation: RI occurs when new learning overwrites or alters previously stored memories, particularly when the new and old information are similar in nature.

Example: After learning French vocabulary, you may struggle to remember Spanish words that you previously knew, as the French words interfere with the retrieval of the Spanish ones.

Evidence: Müller and Pilzecker (1900) conducted studies in which participants learned lists of nonsense syllables. They found that recall was poorer when participants learned a second list shortly after the first, suggesting that the new learning interfered with their ability to remember the earlier information.

Evaluation of Interference Theory

Strengths:

  • Supporting Evidence: There is substantial experimental evidence for interference theory, such as studies on word lists and nonsense syllables, supporting the idea that interference can cause forgetting.
  • Real-Life Application: Interference theory has practical applications, especially in areas where similar information must be learned in succession, such as in language learning or educational settings.

Limitations:

  • Limited Scope: Interference theory explains only a specific type of forgetting – primarily when learning similar information in a short time. It may not fully account for forgetting in other contexts.
  • Artificial Studies: Much of the supporting research involves artificial materials, such as nonsense syllables or word lists, which may lack ecological validity and do not reflect everyday memory use.
  • Retrieval Failure Due to Absence of Cues
  • Retrieval failure is an explanation for forgetting that occurs when memory cues are unavailable. It suggests that information is present in the memory store but cannot be accessed without the appropriate cues. Retrieval failure is often referred to as cue-dependent forgetting and is particularly likely when memories were encoded in a specific context or state.

Types of Cues in Retrieval

Cues are triggers or hints that aid in the retrieval of information. They can be:

Context-Dependent Cues: Cues related to the external environment in which the memory was encoded.

State-Dependent Cues: Cues related to the internal state (such as mood or physiological state) of the person when the memory was formed.

Context-Dependent Forgetting

Context-dependent forgetting occurs when we are unable to retrieve a memory because we are not in the same environment or context where the memory was initially encoded. The original context acts as a cue for recall, and if we are in a different setting, the absence of this cue can lead to retrieval failure.

Key Features:

Explanation: When information is encoded, details of the environment (such as sights, sounds, or smells) may become part of the memory trace. If these context cues are absent, retrieval can be more difficult.

Example: You might struggle to remember specific facts you learned in a classroom if you are trying to recall them in a different room.

Evidence: Godden and Baddeley (1975) conducted a study with divers who learned lists of words either underwater or on land. They found that recall was better when the learning and recall contexts matched (i.e., both underwater or both on land), illustrating the impact of context-dependent cues on memory retrieval.

State-Dependent Forgetting

State-dependent forgetting occurs when a person’s internal physiological or emotional state at the time of encoding differs from their state at the time of recall. The internal state becomes part of the memory trace, and changes in this state may result in retrieval failure.

Key Features:

Explanation: Emotional or physiological states (such as mood, intoxication, or arousal level) can serve as cues, aiding recall when they are the same during learning and retrieval.

Example: If you learned information while feeling anxious, you may remember it better when experiencing similar levels of anxiety.

Evidence: Carter and Cassaday (1998) gave participants an anti-histamine drug to create a drowsy internal state. They found that recall was better when the participants’ internal states matched during learning and recall (both drugged or both sober).

Evaluation of Retrieval Failure

Strengths:

  • Real-World Applications: Retrieval failure theory has practical applications in improving memory recall, such as revisiting a learning environment or recreating specific cues during revision to enhance memory.
  • Supporting Evidence: Both laboratory and naturalistic studies support retrieval failure theory, such as research on context- and state-dependent forgetting, lending credibility to this explanation.

Limitations:

  • Role of Other Factors: Retrieval failure does not account for forgetting caused by decay or interference, which may also contribute to retrieval difficulties.
  • Context Effects Are Limited: Some researchers argue that the effects of context are limited to specific types of memory tasks, as certain information can be recalled effectively regardless of context if it is deeply processed or meaningful.

Summary of Explanations for Forgetting

TheoryDescriptionKey Features and Evidence
Inferference TheoryForgetting occurs because memories interfere with each other, especially when similar.

Proactive Interference: Old Memories disrupt new ones.

Retroactive Interference: New memories disrupt old ones. 

Evidence: Underwood (1957) on proactive interference; Muller & Pilzecker (1900) on retroactive interference. 

Retrieval FailureForgetting occurs due to the absence of cues that were present at encoding.

Context-Dependent Cues: Environmental cues that aid recall.

State-Dependent Cues: Internal states that aid recall.

Evidence: Godden & Baddeley (1975) on context cues; Carter & Cassaday (1998) on state. 

These explanations offer different perspectives on the causes of forgetting and highlight the importance of various factors, including interference from similar memories and the absence of helpful cues, in the retrieval of stored information.

sign up to revision world banner
NTU Brackenhurst College Open Day
Slot